THE IMPLICATIONS OF SINGLE- CELL PHYSIOLOGY FOR PERCEPTION How are cells in our brain likely to respond to some everyday stimulus, such as this kidney-shaped uniform blob? In the visual cortex, only a select set of cells will show any interest. The fact that a cell in the brain responds to visual stimuli does not guarantee that it plays a direct part in perception. For example, many structures in the brainstem that are primarily visual have to do only with eye movements, pupillary constriction, or focusing by means of the lens. We can nevertheless be reasonably sure that the cells I described in this chapter have a lot to do with perception. As I mentioned at the outset, destroying any small piece of our striate cortex produces blindness in some small part of our visual world, and damaging the striate cortex has the same result in the monkey. In the cat things are not so simple: a cat with its striate cortex removed can see, though less well. Other parts of the brain, such as the superior colliculus, may play a relatively more important part in a cat's perception than they do in the primate's. Lower vertebrates, such as frogs and turtles, have nothing quite like our cortex, yet no one would contend that they are blind. We can now say with some confidence what any one of these cortical cells is likely to be doing in response to a natural scene. The majority of cortical cells respond badly to diffuse light but well to appropriately oriented lines. Thus for the kidney shape shown in the illustration to the left, such a cell will fire if and only if its receptive field is cut in the right orientation by the borders. Cells whose receptive fields are inside the borders will be unaffected; they will continue to fire at their spontaneous rate, oblivious to the presence or absence of the form.